Skip to main content

Disturbed theta and gamma coupling as a potential mechanism for visuospatial working memory dysfunction in people with schizophrenia

Abstract

Working memory (WM) deficits have been repeatedly observed in people with schizophrenia (PSZ) and their unaffected biological relatives (REL). Given the apparent association with genetic liability for schizophrenia, WM deficits have been proposed as a potential endophenotype for the disorder. Abnormal neural responses during WM performance have likewise been observed in PSZ and REL and may reflect the expression of genetic liability for schizophrenia in brain function. Relatively recent investigations have examined the role of neural oscillatory activity during visuospatial WM function in healthy people, as well as dysfunction in psychopathology. This research was in part motivated by a neural model of WM proposed by Lisman and Idiart (Science 267:1512–1515, 1995) that delineated a mechanism for representing multiple stimuli within WM through systematic interactions between neural oscillations in the theta- and gamma- frequency ranges. Aberrant oscillatory activity in theta and gamma frequency ranges has since been proposed as a potential underlying factor in WM dysfunction in PSZ and REL. The experimental evidence derived from studies of healthy people that pertains to the theta-gamma model of WM is reviewed herein. Although scarce, direct examinations of theta-gamma interactions in PSZ and REL are likewise reviewed in addition to reports of separate deficits in theta and gamma frequencies observed in PSZ during WM. The implications of theta and gamma oscillatory deficits reported in PSZ are discussed in the context of the Lisman and Idiart (Science 267:1512–1515, 1995) model, as well as how these deficits may result in aberrant theta-gamma interactions that give rise to visuospatial WM dysfunction. Given evidence supporting the Lisman and Idiart (Science 267:1512–1515, 1995) model of theta-gamma interactions in WM and the lack of direct exploration of the model in schizophrenia, there is an imperative to carry out formal testing of theta-gamma interactions in PSZ and REL during WM.

Background

Visuospatial working memory (WM), comprised by the coordination of processing, maintaining, manipulating, and retrieving spatial and visual information within the brain [1], has been implicated in a variety of tasks necessary for the navigation of day-to-day life, including wayfinding [2] and visuomotor learning [3, 4]. Prominent WM deficits have been demonstrated across multiple sensory modalities in people with schizophrenia (PSZ), including in the visuospatial realm ([5 reviewed in 6]). Furthermore, better visuospatial WM function in PSZ has been shown to predict better functional and social outcomes [7, 8]. Spatial WM deficits are likewise observed in unaffected first-degree relatives of PSZ [911], suggesting they may represent genetic liability for schizophrenia and thus constitute a potential endophenotype for the disorder [1215].

Electroencephalography (EEG) has been widely used to investigate the neural underpinnings of WM processes, traditionally in the form of event-related potential (ERP) studies (reviewed in [16]). PSZ have been found to demonstrate deficits in ERPs elicited from WM tasks [1719] and tasks probing sustained attention [2024], among others; similar deficits have been observed in first-degree relatives of people with the disorder [22, 25, 26]. More recently, EEG research has examined oscillatory activity in the brain associated with WM. Oscillatory activity in the brain, which arises from the rhythmic activation of large numbers of synapses [27], is crucial to coordinated activity in the normally functioning brain [28]. PSZ demonstrate many abnormalities in oscillatory activity, which interfere with efficient integration of activity within the brain and may contribute to the prominent cognitive and functional deficits observed in the disorder [2729].

Generally speaking, high frequency oscillations are associated with synchronization in local cortical networks, and lower frequency oscillations with synchronization between brain structures/across greater distances [28]. Oscillations in the gamma-band (25–100 Hz) and the theta-band (4–8 Hz) in particular have been widely implicated in episodic memory and WM alike (reviewed in [30]). Furthermore, the synchronized superimposition of gamma cycles on oscillatory theta activity has been proposed as a mechanism for the representation of multiple items in WM, a mechanism termed the “theta/gamma code” [31]. Multi-item WM has been associated with phase-phase coupling (see Fig. 3) between theta and gamma frequencies in healthy people (e.g., [32]). In addition, theta-gamma phase-amplitude coupling (Fig. 3) has been observed in the hippocampus [33, 34] as well as from scalp EEG [35, 36] in healthy humans and, in both cases, has been predictive of WM performance [3336]. Given these findings, abnormal theta-gamma coupling has been proposed as a mechanism for impaired WM function in PSZ [37, 38], though this hypothesis has yet to be formally investigated.

Much research has already shown abnormalities in the gamma- and theta- bands in PSZ, suggesting dysfunction in neural integration on local and global scales. The present review examines gamma and theta abnormalities during WM in PSZ in the context of the theta/gamma neural code. Though the theta/gamma code has not been formally investigated in schizophrenia to date, the mechanism and hypothesized utility of the superimposition of gamma cycles on theta cycles in relation to WM in healthy people will be reviewed. Subsequently, this review will recount observed abnormalities in PSZ for activity in the gamma-band, theta-band, and interactions between the two during WM, discussing how such abnormalities may interfere with efficient theta/gamma coding in schizophrenia. Through integration of current findings regarding abnormalities in gamma- and theta-band oscillations in schizophrenia during WM, the present review will attempt to elucidate the potential implications of these abnormalities with respect to the theta/gamma neural code.

The superimposition of gamma-band oscillations on theta oscillations: the theta/gamma neural code

The theory of a theta/gamma code as first posited by Lisman and Idiart [39] and elaborated elsewhere (e.g., [40]) offered three primary, interrelated proposals: 1) that systematic coupling of oscillatory activity of different frequencies within the brain is meaningful, 2) that this coupling serves to form a temporal code in which distinct pieces of information are associated with simultaneous activity across several cells at particular phases within a cycle of theta, and 3) that the theta/gamma code model can explain central features of WM. In an effort to support their first two hypotheses regarding the importance of cross-frequency coupling as a temporal code, the authors first addressed the plausibility of oscillatory activity as a sustained temporal organizer. The neuromodulators acetylcholine and serotonin have been shown to be released in the brain during periods of oscillatory activity [41, 42]. In the presence of these neuromodulators, firing of neuronal cells induces a period of membrane afterdepolarization (ADP) rather than the typical afterhyperpolarization, leading to a transient increase in cell excitability [4345]. Building off these findings, the authors used computer simulations to show that the duration of ADP was on the time scale of oscillations in the alpha-theta range (5 to 12 Hz), and that such ADP could be propagated for many cycles. Thus, a single excitatory input could lead to sustained firing on subsequent oscillatory cycles, potentially serving a storage function (Fig. 1a; [39]). Furthermore, the authors noted that in Sternberg’s [46] classic work on serial scanning in WM, the addition of each stimulus to the string of stimuli to be recalled resulted in an increase in reaction time of roughly 38 ms—an increase corresponding to the cycle of a neural oscillation in the beta-gamma range. Jensen and Lisman [47] elaborated on this observation, showing that the theta-gamma code model could effectively account for reaction time data and serial position effects reported by Sternberg [46, 48].Footnote 1 Roughly seven cycles in the beta-gamma range could be superimposed on a lower frequency cycle in the alpha-theta range—such as those cycles induced by the ADP—which Lisman and Idiart [39] emphasized corresponded to the canonical average capacity of WM determined by Miller [49]. Thus, Lisman and Idiart [39] demonstrated the potential for theta-gamma cross-frequency coupling to serve an ongoing, organizing function.

Fig. 1
figure 1

a The afterdepolarization (ADP) allows information storage in a single cell. The neuron receives a suprathreshold informational input and a second, subthreshold input that induces the membrane potential to oscillate at theta frequency (negative phase due to inhibition). Simulations show membrane potential before and after informational input (arrowhead). b Network in which pyramidal cells make converging excitatory synapses onto an inhibitory interneuron that produces feedback inhibition of pyramidal cells. c The network can maintain the firing and correct phase of seven groups of cells that are active during different subcycles of the low-frequency oscillation. Each trace illustrates the synchronous firing of a group of cells whose spatial pattern encodes the memory of a letter. The dashed lines during the second and fourth theta cycles show the different subcycles. The limited memory capacity of the network is demonstrated by its failure to store eight memories. Input of the memory X is successful (arrowhead), but R is lost. d If feedback inhibition is removed (arrowhead), the “40-hz” oscillation and phase information is rapidly lost. The two traces represent two of the seven memories stored in the network. A small phase difference (too small to be shown) persists for one cycle after removal of inhibition. Reproduced from [39]

In their discussion of the temporal properties of theta and gamma oscillations in relation to Sternberg’s classic studies [46, 48], Lisman and Idiart [39] transition to their final proposal regarding the role of the theta/gamma code in WM. The authors demonstrated through simulation how differing, non-overlapping stimuli or “memories” ([39], p. 1515) could be stored through systematic variation in the phases of the different high-frequency subcycles, with each stimulus being represented by different groups of cells that maintain the stimulus by firing simultaneously at a particular high-frequency subcycle within the nesting lower frequency cycle (Fig. 1c). Thus, particular stimuli or memories are represented by the particular spatial pattern of a group of cells, or neural ensemble [37], these memories are propagated or maintained through interactions between oscillatory activity in the theta and gamma frequencies, and sequential information regarding these memories are linked to the particular gamma subcycle within the nesting theta cycle a given memory’s neural ensemble produces (or perhaps more accurately, the phase offset between the neural ensemble’s gamma cycle and the nesting theta cycle; [37, 39]). The theta-gamma neural code as proposed by Lisman and Idiart [39] represents a theoretical framework which, at its inception, integrated a number of experimental findings to provide a plausible model for the storage of multiple items in WM. This framework, which has since been further developed [e.g., 31, 37], has served as the basis for a substantial body of experimentation over the past twenty years, the vast majority of which has produced results supporting the Lisman and Idiart [39] model. A brief review of this sizeable body of literature follows, focused on studies probing WM function.

Empirical support for the theta-gamma neural code

The experimental support for Lisman and Idiart’s [39] model of the theta-gamma neural code is now widespread. In addition to other supporting observations, Lisman and Idiart [39] cited as some of the most significant early support for the theta-gamma neural code model the work of O’Keefe and Recce [50] (later clarified by [51]). These researchers examined pyramidal cells from the hippocampus of rats known as place cells, which fire bursts of spikes as the animal moves through a particular area in its environment termed the place field of that cell [50]. Concurrent to these bursts, theta band activity also occurs in the hippocampal EEG as a rat relocates itself in its environment. Though phase correlations between theta-band activity and bursts of hippocampal activity had then been well-established [5255], the nature of the relationship was not well clarified. Using a combination of single unit and EEG recordings from hippocampal CA1 and CA3 place cells, O’Keefe and Recce [50] showed that for a single run through the field of a given place cell on a linear track, the cell fired bursts of spikes at progressively earlier parts of the of the concomitant theta cycle, a phenomenon termed “phase precession” ([51], p. 149; Fig. 2). This finding was taken to indicate that the hippocampus may use a neural code in which theta phase conveys important information [31]—namely, the theta-gamma neural code that Lisman and Idiart [39] proposed. Skaggs and colleagues [51] replicated and elaborated this finding to demonstrate that the phase precession effect was indeed robust throughout the rat hippocampus, and observed as animals moved through two-dimensional in addition to one-dimensional space. More significantly, the researchers emphasize the support their findings lend to the notion that the theta phase-offset at which a place cell fires carries important spatial information [51]. A considerable body of work has shown that this spiking in the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex is time locked to local gamma-activity, such that firing is effectively limited to discrete, gamma-locked time windows (see [56] and [57]). Bieri, Bobbitt, and Colgin [58] similarly observed theta-gamma phase precession in rat CA1 hippocampal cells and suggested differential relationships between coupling between high- and low-gamma signals. Phase precession has likewise been observed in entorhinal grid cells [59], supporting the organization of multi-item WM by way of theta- and gamma- cycles as by Lisman and Idiart’s [39] model and its elaborations. Phase-precession is further reviewed in [56].

Much subsequent experimentation has added to the support for Lisman and Idiart’s [39] model, demonstrating systematic relationships between theta- and gamma- oscillatory activity. These relationships are most frequently presented in one of two forms: phase-phase coupling (also called phase-synchrony, or simply phase-coupling), in which a given phase angle of a cycle of ongoing theta-activity tends to occur at a given phase angle of concurrent gamma-activity; and phase-amplitude coupling, in which the amplitude of gamma activity is modulated by the phase of concurrent theta-oscillations (Fig. 3). Though these types of coupling are most frequently reported separately, it has been suggested that they are likely two expressions of a single phenomenon [60, 61].

Fig. 2
figure 2

Extraction of the firing phase shift for each spike during a single run through the field of a CA3 place cell on the linear track. a Each firing of cell 3 as a single vertical line during the 1 s of data. b Phase of each spike relative to the theta cycle within which it falls as a horizontal line. c Hippocampal theta activity recorded at the same time as the single unit. d Result of the theta template matching algorithm. Note that the amplitude and the time between onsets of each template match varies to fit the variations in the theta. The small vertical ticks above the electroencephalogram (EEG) and below the template fits mark the beginning of each theta cycle. The cell clearly fires six bursts of spikes during this run through the place field. Comparison of each burst with the concomitant theta wave shows that each successive burst fires on an earlier part of the theta. This is shown clearly by the descending staircase of the phase correlates in B. EEG voltage in C is + 1 to - 1 mV. Total time between marks on the x axis is 1 s. Reproduced from [50]

Fig. 3
figure 3

Schematic overview of two cases in which oscillatory activity of a higher-frequency oscillation may be related to the phase of a lower-frequency oscillation. Dark and light boxes separate consecutive cycles of the lower-frequency oscillation. The lower-frequency oscillation of fluctuating amplitude (top) shows phase-amplitude coupling with a higher-frequency oscillation of fluctuating amplitude (middle). In this example, amplitudes of the higher-frequency oscillation are maximal during the up-phase of the lower-frequency oscillation. A higher-frequency oscillation of stable amplitude (bottom), shows phase-phase coupling with the low-frequency oscillation. Here, peaks of the higher-frequency oscillation always coincide with the same phase values of the lower-frequency oscillations. Reproduced from [79]

Findings of both types of coupling are summarized in Tables 1 and 2. Review of the animal studies in Table 1 reveals well-documented increases of theta-gamma coupling using intracranial recordings from the entorhinal-hippocampal system of rodents during performance of memory-focused tasks (e.g., Table 1, references i-xi). Theta-gamma coupling is likewise observed using intracranial recordings from the entorhinal cortex and hippocampus during free exploration, sleep and anesthetization (e.g., xii-xxvi) as well as from in vitro isolated samples (e.g., xxvii-xxix) in rats and mice. Primate studies have also revealed theta-gamma coupling in auditory cortex (xxx) during a passive listening task as well as from anterior cingulate cortex during an attention task (xxxi). Thus, WM processes are associated with increased theta-gamma coupling in the animal literature, though coupling observed in less-memory-dependent paradigms indicates that theta-gamma coupling may represent a more widespread means of communication in the brains of rodents and primates.

Table 1 Animal findings regarding theta-gamma coupling (TGC)

Table 2 reports findings of theta-gamma coupling in humans. Here, we see intracranial recordings utilized to demonstrate phase-phase coupling (e.g., Table 2, references i, vii) as well as phase-amplitude coupling (e.g., ii-vi) between theta- and gamma- activity in hippocampal areas as well as throughout the cortex. Furthermore, scalp-level recordings have shown phase-phase theta-gamma coupling in humans over primarily frontal, parietal and occipital areas (e.g., viii-xi, xv), as well as phase-amplitude coupling over much of the scalp, including prefrontal, occipital and parietal sites (e.g. xi, xiii, xiv). Notably, one recent experiment temporarily increased individuals’ short-term memory capacities by using transcranial alternating current stimulation to slow their theta cycles (xvi), while another used MEG to show progressively later peaks in gamma activity relative to concurrent theta activity for progressively later memory items in an encoding sequence (xvii). In addition, measures of theta-gamma coupling have similarly been related to WM performance in experimentation with humans (e.g., vii, xi, xiii, xv). Thus, theta-gamma coupling has been repeatedly associated with WM processes in both the animal and human literature.

Table 2 Human findings regarding theta-gamma coupling (TGC)

The role of abnormal theta-/gamma- oscillations in schizophrenia

Given the well-documented importance of theta-gamma modulations to memory processes in animal and human studies alike, abnormal interactions between theta- and gamma-oscillations have been proposed to explain the prominent WM deficits observed in PSZ [37, 38]. In particular, predictive relationships demonstrated between indices of theta-gamma coupling and WM capacity [32, 34] as well as performance on WM tasks [36, 107] in healthy people suggest that impaired WM performance observed in PSZ may be attributed to deficient theta-gamma coupling. Formal investigation of this proposal has been scarce to date. As such, in addition to the few direct examinations of theta-gamma coupling in PSZ, demonstrated abnormalities in theta- and gamma- oscillations independently are likewise reported, and their potential relevance to interactions between the two frequency bands is discussed.

Theta-gamma coupling in people with schizophrenia

Few studies have directly examined interactions between theta- and gamma- activity in PSZ. Allen and colleagues [62] examined phase-amplitude coupling between low- and high- frequencies using independent component analysis (ICA) of EEG recorded from PSZ during performance of an auditory oddball task. In this ICA, data for comodulation between various frequencies were decomposed into separate components based on spectral and spatial composition. One such component showing widespread coupling between lower-frequency phases and higher-frequency amplitudes across the entire scalp was less prominent in PSZ data compared to that of healthy controls, and another component depicting modulation of high frequency amplitude by low frequency phase at frontotemporal sites was more prominent for PSZ than controls. Another component revealed strong theta-gamma coupling over occipital-parietal areas, but the loading parameters for said component did not differ between groups (e.g., the component was equivalently strong in PSZ and healthy controls). First-degree relatives of people with the disorder were included in this study and showed coupling intermediate to that of PSZ and healthy controls, though their data were not tested statistically due to power limitations. The authors interpret their results to indicate that abnormal cross-frequency coupling may represent an endophenotype for schizophrenia, particularly in light of genetic associations they report for genotypes for certain genes and loading parameters for various independent components in PSZ. However, though patterns of cross-frequency coupling were altered in PSZ, theta-gamma coupling is still prominent, especially over posterior sites. As such, theta-gamma coupling may not be significantly altered in the disorder, at least in the context of novelty detection. No correlations between measures of cross-frequency coupling and performance were observed [62]. Thus, this study provides evidence for preserved theta-gamma phase-amplitude coupling in PSZ; however, given the minimal role WM plays in the oddball paradigm, the theta-gamma interactions examined in this study may be distinct from those related to WM.

The only other published work examining theta-gamma cross-frequency interactions in PSZ comes from Kirihara and colleagues [63]. Using scalp EEG recorded during presentation of 40-hz steady state auditory stimuli, the authors found reduced intertrial phase coherence, increased theta amplitude, and undisturbed theta-gamma phase-amplitude coupling in PSZ relative to healthy controls. The authors interpret these findings to indicate a preserved hierarchical organization of theta and gamma activity in PSZ despite abnormalities in theta- and gamma- activity independently [63]. Notably, the simple auditory processing task used in this study does not tap memory processes, for which theta-gamma interactions have been shown to essential and greatly impaired in PSZ; thus, the reported intact theta-gamma coupling should not be interpreted as indicative of preserved coupling at large.

Two additional studies investigated theta-gamma coupling under influence of ketamine, an NMDAR antagonist which is used as a pharmacological model of schizophrenia in both animals and humans. Neymotin and colleagues [64] used a computer model of hippocampal CA3 to investigate the effect of the application of simulated ketamine. Ketamine administration disrupted theta-gamma coupling in the simulation, apparently due to its antagonistic effect on NMDA receptors on oriens-lacunosum moleculare cells (OLM); normal coupling was restored via injection of a continuous, depolarizing current into these OLM cells, suggesting a potential intervention for treating deficits in WM and other cognitive processes in PSZ. Similarly, Caixeta and colleagues [65] found that ketamine altered theta-gamma coupling in the hippocampus of rats, again suggesting the importance of theta-gamma interactions to cognitive deficits in PSZ. Still, theta-gamma coupling in the context of memory function in PSZ has not yet been directly examined.

Theta- and gamma- band abnormalities in people with schizophrenia

Though research on theta-gamma coupling in schizophrenia is sparse, findings from the individual bands support the notion of WM impairments in PSZ stemming from aberrant theta-gamma interactions. Abnormalities in theta- and gamma-activity during WM reported in PSZ are summarized in Table 3. These abnormalities include deficits in theta-synchrony (e.g., Table 3, reference iv) and aberrations in overall theta-amplitudes (e.g., i-iv) as well as modulations in response to task demands (e.g., i, iv), though results are variable regarding whether PSZ show enhanced or reduced amplitudes/modulation as compared with controls. Despite this variability, all studies show a failure of PSZ to modulate theta amplitudes with changes in task demands compared to controls, suggesting an inefficient allocation of cognitive resources (suggested by i, iii, iv) and/or deficiencies in neural responses associated with sequential presentation of stimuli (suggested by ii). Amplitudes of gamma-activity during WM have similarly been found to be abnormal in PSZ (e.g., v-ix), though findings are again mixed as to whether these abnormalities are enhancements or reductions compared to controls; as with theta, however, PSZ frequently fail to modulate (e.g., vi, viii-ix) or abnormally modulate (vii) gamma responses with changes in task demands. Again, these findings suggest that PSZ either lack or misallocate cognitive resources as compared to healthy controls. Discrepancies in whether oscillatory responses are increased or decreased in PSZ relative to controls have many potential sources, including heterogeneity in symptomatology, differences in stage of illness (e.g., early onset and/or first-episode patients), electrode site selection (e.g., single channel versus pooled sites) or means of collection (e.g., MEG vs. EEG). The literature nonetheless demonstrates consistent reports of abnormalities in theta- and gamma-band activity for PSZ as compared to healthy controls during WM processes, particularly in response to changes in task conditions related to WM function.

Table 3 Theta and gamma abnormalities in people with schizophrenia (PSZ) during working memory processes

Though the present review focuses on the relation of theta- and gamma-oscillations to WM dysfunction in PSZ, the role of oscillatory activity in other frequencies and in relation to other cognitive functions has been reviewed elsewhere [28, 38, 66, 67]. Thus, abnormalities in oscillatory activity in PSZ do not appear to be limited to theta- and gamma-band activity, and may contribute to a wide variety of symptomatology observed in the disorder.

Relevance of abnormalities in theta- and gamma- activity in people with schizophrenia

PSZ demonstrate abnormalities in theta- and gamma- band activity during WM paradigms. Though no studies of WM in PSZ have examined coupling between theta- and gamma- activities, the ramifications of deficiencies within each of these bands independently on their interaction are easily ascertained. Inconsistencies in phase, as indexed by abnormal phase synchrony measures, would significantly interfere with any meaningful coordination between signals of differing frequencies. Similarly, amplitude deficiencies suggest abnormal generation of neural oscillations, be it through activation of fewer cells or impaired coordination of cellular assemblies that would function more cohesively in the healthy brain. Regardless, less reliable generators of oscillatory activity will limit the unitary functioning of cell assemblies that is necessary for coordination of signals that compose the theta-gamma neural code. Although coupling between theta- and gamma- activity in PSZ has been investigated, the few examinations of interactions between these bands in PSZ have not involved WM directly. Though theta-gamma coupling has been observed during processes other than WM in healthy people, the bulk of the literature ties it to memory processes; thus, further investigation is needed to clarify whether theta-gamma interactions are indeed abnormal during WM processes in PSZ, and whether those abnormalities explain behavioral performance deficits associated with the disorder.

Conclusions: the theta-gamma neural code and visuospatial WM in schizophrenia

Lisman and Idiart’s [39] model positing a theta/gamma neural code as a means for representing multiple items in WM has generated a significant body of literature, and has been implicated in an even greater collection of research. A considerable amount of evidence has been accumulated and overwhelmingly supports the notions advanced by the model, namely that the interaction between theta- and gamma- oscillations communicate meaningful information within the brain in terms of memory and potentially other cognitive processes. Recent experimentation with humans has supported the model more directly: gamma activity associated with particular sequentially presented stimuli has been found to peak at different phases of concurrent theta activity in accordance with stimulus position within the sequence, and electrical stimulation in the gamma frequency range has shown to improve WM performance when administered at particular phases of theta. Regardless of how generalized the theta/gamma code is within the brain, it is difficult to dispute the notion that theta-gamma interactions are central features of WM processes in humans and animals. Furthermore, measures of the strength of coupling between the two bands have repeatedly shown to predict WM performance, supporting the notion that theta-/gamma- interaction and memory are causally linked.

Despite this sizeable body of evidence supporting the importance of theta-gamma coupling to WM processes in humans and animals, few studies have examined interactions between the bands in PSZ [62, 63], and none in the context of WM. As such, much additional experimentation is required to determine whether deficient theta-gamma coupling may explain WM deficits in PSZ. Notably, though several studies have demonstrated predictive relationships between measures of coupling and WM ability in healthy people, few have compared the predictive ability of theta-gamma coupling to other indices of oscillatory activity, including measures of power and/or phase synchrony within theta-, gamma-, and other frequency bands. One notable exception is [32], who found an index of alpha-band power predicted WM capacity better than indices of theta-gamma coupling; the researchers interpreted this alpha measure as an index of the suppression of irrelevant information, a mechanism arguably distinct from that being presently examined. Regardless, the logical question is thus whether deficient theta-gamma coupling truly explains WM deficiencies, or if it is merely an important correlate of some other primary cause. Addressing such a question is an important next step in further asserting the causal role of theta-gamma coupling in WM processes as well as the veracity of Lisman and Idiart’s [39] theta/gamma neural code.Footnote 2

Despite the unanswered questions that remain, considerable evidence has been found to support the theory of a theta/gamma code. Much of this evidence is derived from examinations of spatial WM processes in both rodents and humans. Given the well documented deficits in visuospatial WM in PSZ [6] and their relatives [911], a clear and testable hypothesis is that abnormal interactions between theta- and gamma-activity explains these deficits in visuospatial WM and WM at large. The hypothesis is supported by the considerable body of literature demonstrating abnormalities in theta- and gamma-bands individually within PSZ, including in connection with WM. Examination of theta-gamma coupling during WM in PSZ may yield a better understanding of mechanisms underlying the prominent WM deficits observed in people with the disorder. Lisman and Idiart’s [39] theta-gamma neural code provides a framework for testing a specific source of WM dysfunction in PSZ. Using the Lisman and Idiart [39] model as a guide, we might expect to find generally reduced phase-phase coupling between theta- and gamma-activity during performance of tasks probing WM in PSZ. Theta-gamma coupling in healthy people has been observed prominently over prefrontal and posterior regions in scalp level recordings [32, 35, 36, 6870]. Thus, deficits in synchrony between the two bands observed over anterior and posterior regions in scalp recordings, as well as in theta-gamma synchrony between the two areas, would particularly support the notion that deficits in the temporal alignment of theta- and gamma- activities are inextricably linked to WM function in the disorder. Furthermore, such synchrony deficits would mirror reports of inefficiencies in prefrontal areas [71] as well as deficient functional connectivity between prefrontal and posterior areas associated with WM processes observed in PSZ using functional MRI [72, 73], and may represent the same dysfunction. Theta-gamma interactions may likewise be disrupted in the hippocampus in PSZ, given the predominance of reported theta-gamma coupling during WM processes in animals and healthy people (e.g., Table 1) and observations that structural abnormalities in the hippocampus are prominent in schizophrenia [74] and observed even in cases of early-onset [75]. As activity from the hippocampus is difficult to observe using scalp level recordings (see [76]), intracranial EEG would be necessary to investigate hippocampal theta-gamma coupling deficits in PSZ; however, were such studies conducted, the Lisman and Idiart [39] model in combination with previous findings suggest abnormalities in theta-gamma coupling would be likely.

Furthermore, investigations of theta-gamma abnormalities during spatial WM processing in PSZ would be more informative if they included a task with a sequential presentation of stimuli. The Lisman and Idiart [39] model predicts different theta-phase preferences for different stimuli, represented by individual gamma cycles superimposed upon a theta cycle, in a memory sequence. The discussed phenomenon of phase-precession observed in rodents [50, 51, 56, 58, 59] represents one of the primary pieces of evidence supporting the theory and contributing to its continued consideration, and recent work has shown progressive shifts in gamma activity based on stimulus sequence [77]. However, paradigms that would allow for investigation of differential phase-preferences for individual stimuli in a spatial memory sequence are decidedly absent from human study. Thus, utilization of a paradigm featuring a sequential presentation of visuospatial stimuli would allow for a direct examination of whether phase-preferences are present in humans, and/or disrupted in PSZ.

In light of the dearth of literature testing the role of theta-gamma coupling in WM deficits in PSZ and their relatives, further study into these interactions between theta- and gamma-activity associated with WM in people genetically liable for schizophrenia is strongly warranted. Lisman and Idiart’s [39] model provides a concrete framework for the design of an ideal investigation of these processes in PSZ, and suggests that deficits in theta-gamma interactions are likely to be observed in such experimentation. Better understanding of how neural oscillations factor into visuospatial WM deficits in the disorder could help inform interventions for improving memory performance, particularly in light of continually developing techniques of neuromodulation that have been shown to improve cognitive function using oscillatory stimulation (e.g., [78]).

Notes

  1. Sternberg himself recently reviewed the compatibility of Lisman and Idiart’s [39] model with high-speed scanning research; see [120].

  2. Much of this paragraph is derived from insightful commentary from one of the paper’s reviewers, which is greatly appreciated.

Abbreviations

PSZ:

People with schizophrenia

REL:

Unaffected first-degree biological relatives of people with schizophrenia

EEG:

Electroencephalography

ERP:

Event-related potential

ADP:

Afterdepolarization

ICA:

Independent component analysis

MRI:

Magnetic resonance imaging

TGC:

Theta-gamma coupling

iEEG:

Intracranial EEG

LFP:

Local field potential

CSD:

Current source density

EC:

Entorhinal cortex

HFO:

High-frequency oscillation

ACC:

Anterior cingulate cortex

PFC:

Prefrontal cortex

MEG:

Magnetoencephalography

WM:

Working memory

References

  1. De Beni R, Pazzaglia F, Gyselinck V, Meneghetti C. Visuospatial working memory and mental representation of spatial descriptions. Eur J Cogn Psychol. 2005;17:77–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Meilinger T, Knauff M, Bülthoff HH. Working memory in wayfinding—A dual task experiment in a virtual city. Cognit Sci. 2008;32:755–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Anguera JA, Reuter-Lorenz PA, Willingham DT, Seidler RD. Contributions of spatial working memory to visuomotor learning. J Cogn Neurosci. 2010;22:1917–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Uresti-Cabrera LA, Diaz R, Vaca-Palomares I, Fernandez-Ruiz J. The effect of spatial working memory deterioration on strategic visuomotor learning across aging. Behav Neurol. 2015;2015:1–7.

  5. Conklin HM, Curtis CE, Calkins ME, Iacono WG. Working memory functioning in schizophrenia patients and their first-degree relatives: cognitive functioning shedding light on etiology. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43:930–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Lee J, Park S. Working memory impairments in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. J Abnorm Psychol. 2005;114:599–611.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Goghari VM, Brett C, Tabraham P, Johns L, Valmaggia L, Broome M, Woolley J, Bramon E, Howes O, Byrne M, McGuire P. Spatial working memory ability in individuals at ultra high risk for psychosis. J Psychiatr Res. 2014;50:100–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Takahashi H, Iwase M, Nakahachi T, Sekiyama R, Tabushi K, Kajimoto O, Shimizu A, Takeda M. Spatial working memory deficit correlates with disorganization symptoms and social functioning in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2005;59:453–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Park S, Holzman PS, Goldman-Rakic PS. Spatial working memory deficits in the relatives of schizophrenic patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;52:821–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Pirkola T, Tuulio-Henriksson A, Glahn D, Kieseppä T, Haukka J, Kaprio J, Lӧnnqvist J, Cannon TD. Spatial working memory function in twins with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58:930–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Bachman P, Kim J, Yee CM, Therman S, Manninen M, Lӧnnqvist J, Kaprio J, Huttunen MO, Näätänen R, Cannon TD. Efficiency of working memory encoding in twins discordant for schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2009;174:97–104.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Allen AJ, Griss ME, Folley BS, Hawkins KA, Pearlson GD. Endophenotypes in schizophrenia: a selective review. Schizophr Res. 2009;109:24–37.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Glahn DC, Therman S, Manninen M, Huttunen M, Kaprio J, Lӧnnqvist J, Cannon TD. Spatial working memory as an endophenotype for schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;53:624–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Gottesman II, Gould TD. The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:636–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Snitz BE, MacDonald AW, Carter CS. Cognitive deficits in unaffected first-degree relatives of schizophrenia patients: a meta-analytic review of putative endophenotypes. Schizophr Bull. 2006;32:179–94.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Perez VB, Vogel EK, Luck S, Kappenman E. What ERPs can tell us about working memory, The Oxford Handbook of Event-Related Potential Components. 2012. p. 361–72.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Dias EC, Butler PD, Hoptman MJ, Javitt DC. Early sensory contributions to contextual encoding deficits in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68:654–64.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Haenschel C, Bittner RA, Haertling F, Rotarska-Jagiela A, Maurer K, Singer W, Linden DE. Contribution of impaired early-stage visual processing to working memory dysfunction in adolescents with schizophrenia: a study with event-related potentials and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:1229–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Zhao YL, Tan SP, De Yang F, Wang LL, Feng WF, Chan RC, Gao X, Zhou DF, Li BB, Song CS, et al. Dysfunction in different phases of working memory in schizophrenia: evidence from ERP recordings. Schizophr Res. 2011;133:112–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Davenport ND, Sponheim SR, Stanwyck JJ. Neural anomalies during visual search in schizophrenia patients and unaffected siblings of schizophrenia patients. Schizophr Res. 2006;82:15–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Ergen M, Marbach S, Brand A, Başar-Eroğlu C, Demiralp T. P3 and delta band responses in visual oddball paradigm in schizophrenia. Neurosci Lett. 2008;440:304–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Lee SY, Namkoong K, Cho HH, Song D-H, An SK. Reduced visual P300 amplitudes in individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis and first-episode schizophrenia. Neurosci Lett. 2010;486:156–60.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Oribe N, Hirano Y, Kanba S, del Re EC, Seidman LJ, Mesholam-Gately R, Spencer KM, McCarley RW, Niznikiewicz MA. Early and late stages of visual processing in individuals in prodromal state and first episode schizophrenia: an ERP study. Schizophr Res. 2013;146:95–102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Yeap S, Kelly SP, Sehatpour P, Magno E, Garavan H, Thakore JH, Foxe JJ. Visual sensory processing deficits in Schizophrenia and their relationship to disease state. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2008;258:305–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Sponheim SR, McGuire KA, Stanwyck JJ. Neural anomalies during sustained attention in first-degree biological relatives of schizophrenia patients. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:242–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Yeap S, Kelly SP, Sehatpour P, Magno E, Javitt DC, Garavan H, Thakore JH, Foxe JJ. Early visual sensory deficits as endophenotypes for schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63:1180–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Spellman TJ, Gordon JA. Synchrony in schizophrenia: a window into circuit-level pathophysiology. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2015;30:17–23.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Uhlhaas PJ, Singer W. Abnormal neural oscillations and synchrony in schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11:100–13.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Haenschel C, Bittner RA, Waltz J, Haertling F, Wibral M, Singer W, Linden DE, Rodriguez E. Cortical oscillatory activity is critical for working memory as revealed by deficits in early-onset schizophrenia. J Neurosci. 2009;29:9481–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Nyhus E, Curran T. Functional role of gamma and theta oscillations in episodic memory. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010;34:1023–35.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Lisman JE, Jensen O. The theta-gamma neural code. Neuron. 2013;77:1002–16.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Sauseng P, Klimesch W, Heise KF, Gruber WR, Holz E, Karim AA, Glennon M, Gerloff C, Birbaumer N, Hummel FC. Brain oscillatory substrates of visual short-term memory capacity. Curr Biol. 2009;19:1846–52.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Axmacher N, Henseler MM, Jensen O, Weinreich I, Elger CE, Fell J. Cross-frequency coupling supports multi-item working memory in the human hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2010;107:3228–33.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Chaieb L, Leszczynski M, Axmacher N, Hӧhne M, Elger CE, Fell J. Theta-gamma phase-phase coupling during working memory maintenance in the human hippocampus. Cogn Neurosci. 2015;6:149–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Park JY, Jhung K, Lee J, An SK. Theta-gamma coupling during a working memory task as compared to a simple vigilance task. Neurosci Lett. 2013;532:39–43.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Park JY, Lee Y-R, Lee J. The relationship between theta-gamma coupling and spatial memory ability in older adults. Neurosci Lett. 2011;498:37–41.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Lisman JE, Buzsáki G. A neural coding scheme formed by the combined function of gamma and theta oscillations. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34:974–80.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Moran LV, Hong LE. High vs low frequency neural oscillations in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37:659–63.

  39. Lisman JE, Idiart MA. Storage of 7+/−2 short-term memories in oscillatory subcycles. Science. 1995;267:1512–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Koene RA, Hasselmo ME. First-in-first-out item replacement in a model of short-term memory based on persistent spiking. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:1766–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Bland BH. The physiology and pharmacology of hippocampal formation theta rhythms. Prog Neurobiol. 1986;26:1–54.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Steriade M, Dossi RC, Pare D, Oakson G. Fast oscillations (20–40 Hz) in thalamocortical systems and their potentiation by mesopontine cholinergic nuclei in the cat. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 1991;88:4396–400.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Andrade R. Cell excitation enhances muscarinic cholinergic responses in rat association cortex. Brain Res. 1991;548:81–93.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Caeser M, Brown DA, Gähwiler BH, Knӧpfel T. Characterization of a calcium-dependent current generating a slow afterdepolarization of CA3 pyramidal cells in Rat hippocampal slice cultures. Eur J Neurosci. 1993;5:560–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Storm JF. An after-hyperpolarization of medium duration in rat hippocampal pyramidal cells. J Physiol. 1989;409:171–90.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Sternberg S. High-speed scanning in human memory. Science. 1966;153:652–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Jensen O, Lisman JE. An oscillatory short-term memory buffer model can account for data on the Sternberg task. J Neurosci. 1998;18:10688–99.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Sternberg S. Estimating the Distribution of Additive Reaction-Time Components.0.

  49. Miller GA. The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychol Rev. 1956;63:81–97.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. O’Keefe J, Recce ML. Phase relationship between hippocampal place units and the EEG theta rhythm. Hippocampus. 1993;3:317–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Skaggs WE, McNaughton BL, Wilson MA, Barnes CA. Theta phase precession in hippocampal neuronal populations and the compression of temporal sequences. Hippocampus. 1996;6:149–72.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Fox S, Wolfson S, Ranck Jr J. Hippocampal theta rhythm and the firing of neurons in walking and urethane anesthetized rats. Exp Brain Res. 1986;62:495–508.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Buzsáki G, Leung L-WS, Vanderwolf CH. Cellular bases of hippocampal EEG in the behaving rat. Brain Res Rev. 1983;6:139–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Sinclair BR, Seto MG, Bland BH. Theta-cells in CA1 and dentate layers of hippocampal formation: relations to slow-wave activity and motor behavior in the freely moving rabbit. J Neurophysiol. 1982;48:1214–25.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Otto T, Eichenbaum H, Wible CG, Wiener SI. Learning-related patterns of CA1 spike trains parallel stimulation parameters optimal for inducing hippocampal long-term potentiation. Hippocampus. 1991;1:181–92.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Lisman JE, Redish AD. Prediction, sequences and the hippocampus. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2009;364:1193–201.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Schomburg EW, Fernández-Ruiz A, Mizuseki K, Berényi A, Anastassiou CA, Koch C, Buzsáki G. Theta phase segregation of input-specific gamma patterns in entorhinal-hippocampal networks. Neuron. 2014;84:470–85.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Bieri KW, Bobbitt KN, Colgin LL. Slow and fast gamma rhythms coordinate different spatial coding modes in hippocampal place cells. Neuron. 2014;82:670–81.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. De Almeida L, Idiart M, Villavicencio A, Lisman J. Alternating predictive and short-term memory modes of entorhinal grid cells. Hippocampus. 2012;22:1647–51.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Belluscio MA, Mizuseki K, Schmidt R, Kempter R, Buzsáki G. Cross-frequency phase-phase coupling between theta and gamma oscillations in the hippocampus. J Neurosci. 2012;32:423–35.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Canolty RT, Edwards E, Dalal SS, Soltani M, Nagarajan SS, Kirsch HE, Berger MS, Barbaro NM, Knight RT. High gamma power is phase-locked to theta oscillations in human neocortex. Science. 2006;313:1626–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Allen EA, Liu J, Kiehl KA, Gelernter J, Pearlson GD, Perrone-Bizzozero NI, Calhoun VD. Components of cross-frequency modulation in health and disease. Front Syst Neurosci. 2011;5:1–16.

  63. Kirihara K, Rissling AJ, Swerdlow NR, Braff DL, Light GA. Hierarchical organization of gamma and theta oscillatory dynamics in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;71:873–80.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Neymotin SA, Lazarewicz MT, Sherif M, Contreras D, Finkel LH, Lytton WW. Ketamine disrupts theta modulation of gamma in a computer model of hippocampus. J Neurosci. 2011;31:11733–43.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. Caixeta FV, Cornélio AM, Scheffer-Teixeira R, Ribeiro S, Tort AB. Ketamine alters oscillatory coupling in the hippocampus. Sci Rep. 2013;3:1–10.

  66. Senkowski D, Gallinat J. Dysfunctional Prefrontal Gamma-band Oscillations Reflect Working Memory and Other Cognitive Deficits in Schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2015;77:1010–19.

  67. Pittman-Polletta BR, Kocsis B, Vijayan S, Whittington MA, Kopell NJ. Brain Rhythms Connect Impaired Inhibition to Altered Cognition in Schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2015;77:1020–30.

  68. Holz EM, Glennon M, Prendergast K, Sauseng P. Theta-gamma phase synchronization during memory matching in visual working memory. Neuroimage. 2010;52:326–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Sauseng P, Klimesch W, Gruber WR, Birbaumer N. Cross-frequency phase synchronization: a brain mechanism of memory matching and attention. Neuroimage. 2008;40:308–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Schack B, Vath N, Petsche H, Geissler H-G, Mӧller E. Phase-coupling of theta-gamma EEG rhythms during short-term memory processing. Int J Psychophysiol. 2002;44:143–63.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Potkin S, Turner J, Brown G, McCarthy G, Greve D, Glover G, Manoach D, Belger A, Diaz M, Wible C, Ford J, Mathalon D, Gollub R, Lauriello J, O’Leary D, van Erp T, Toga A, Preda A, Lim K. FBIRN: working memory and DLPFC inefficiency in schizophrenia: the FBIRN study. Schizophr Bull. 2009;35:19–31.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Kang SS, Sponheim SR, Chafee MV, MacDonald AW. Disrupted functional connectivity for controlled visual processing as a basis for impaired spatial working memory in schizophrenia. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:2836–47.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Poppe AB, Carter CS, Minzenberg MJ, MacDonald AW. Task-based functional connectivity as an indicator of genetic liability to schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2015;162:118–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. MacDonald AW, Schulz SC. What we know: findings that every theory of schizophrenia should explain. Schizophr Bull. 2009;35:493–508.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. White T, Cullen K, Rohrer LM, Karatekin C, Luciana M, Schmidt M, Hongwanishkul D, Kumra S, Schulz SC, Lim KO. Limbic structures and networks in children and adolescents with schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34:18–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Mormann F, Fell J, Axmacher N, Weber B, Lehnertz K, Elger CE, Fernández G. Phase/amplitude reset and theta-gamma interaction in the human medial temporal lobe during a continuous word recognition memory task. Hippocampus. 2005;15:890–900.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Heusser AC, Poeppel D, Ezzyat Y, Davachi L. Episodic sequence memory is supported by a theta-gamma phase code. Nat Neurosci. 2016;19:1374–80.

  78. Lee DJ, Gurkoff GG, Izadi A, Berman RF, Ekstrom AD, Muizelaar JP, Lyeth BG, Shahlaie K. Medial septal nucleus theta frequency deep brain stimulation improves spatial working memory after traumatic brain injury. J Neurotrauma. 2013;30:131–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Fell J, Axmacher N. The role of phase synchronization in memory processes. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12:105–18.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Tort AB, Kramer MA, Thorn C, Gibson DJ, Kubota Y, Graybiel AM, Kopell NJ. Dynamic cross-frequency couplings of local field potential oscillations in rat striatum and hippocampus during performance of a T-maze task. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2008;105:20517–22.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  81. Tort AB, Komorowski RW, Manns JR, Kopell NJ, Eichenbaum H. Theta-gamma coupling increases during the learning of item-context associations. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2009;106:20942–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  82. Shirvalkar PR, Rapp PR, Shapiro ML. Bidirectional changes to hippocampal theta-gamma comodulation predict memory for recent spatial episodes. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2010;107:7054–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Cabral HO, Vinck M, Fouquet C, Pennartz CM, Rondi-Reig L, Battaglia FP. Oscillatory dynamics and place field maps reflect hippocampal ensemble processing of sequence and place memory under NMDA receptor control. Neuron. 2014;81:402–15.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Igarashi KM, Lu L, Colgin LL, Moser M-B, Moser EI. Coordination of entorhinal-hippocampal ensemble activity during associative learning. Nature. 2014;510:143–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Nishida H, Takahashi M, Lauwereyns J. Within-session dynamics of theta-gamma coupling and high-frequency oscillations during spatial alternation in rat hippocampal area CA1. Cogn Neurodynamics. 2014;8:363–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  86. Takahashi M, Nishida H, Redish AD, Lauwereyns J. Theta phase shift in spike timing and modulation of gamma oscillation: a dynamic code for spatial alternation during fixation in rat hippocampal area CA1. J Neurophysiol. 2014;111:1601–14.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  87. Trimper JB, Stefanescu RA, Manns JR. Recognition memory and theta-gamma interactions in the hippocampus. Hippocampus. 2014;24:341–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Siegle JH, Wilson MA. Enhancement of encoding and retrieval functions through theta phase-specific manipulation of hippocampus. eLife. 2014;3:1–18.

  89. Soltesz I, Deschenes M. Low-and high-frequency membrane potential oscillations during theta activity in CA1 and CA3 pyramidal neurons of the rat hippocampus under ketamine-xylazine anesthesia. J Neurophysiol. 1993;70:97–116.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Bragin A, Jandó G, Nádasdy Z, Hetke J, Wise K, Buzsáki G. Gamma (40–100 Hz) oscillation in the hippocampus of the behaving rat. J Neurosci. 1995;15:47–60.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Chrobak JJ, Buzsáki G. Gamma oscillations in the entorhinal cortex of the freely behaving rat. J Neurosci. 1998;18:388–98.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Buzsáki G, Buhl D, Harris K, Csicsvari J, Czeh B, Morozov A. Hippocampal network patterns of activity in the mouse. Neuroscience. 2003;116:201–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Csicsvari J, Jamieson B, Wise KD, Buzsáki G. Mechanisms of gamma oscillations in the hippocampus of the behaving rat. Neuron. 2003;37:311–22.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Hentschke H, Perkins MG, Pearce RA, Banks MI. Muscarinic blockade weakens interaction of gamma with theta rhythms in mouse hippocampus. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;26:1642–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Sirota A, Montgomery S, Fujisawa S, Isomura Y, Zugaro M, Buzsáki G. Entrainment of neocortical neurons and gamma oscillations by the hippocampal theta rhythm. Neuron. 2008;60:683–97.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  96. Wulff P, Ponomarenko AA, Bartos M, Korotkova TM, Fuchs EC, Bähner F, Both M, Tort AB, Kopell NJ, Wisden W, Monyer H. Hippocampal theta rhythm and its coupling with gamma oscillations require fast inhibition onto parvalbumin-positive interneurons. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2009;106:3561–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  97. Quilichini P, Sirota A, Buzsáki G. Intrinsic circuit organization and theta-gamma oscillation dynamics in the entorhinal cortex of the rat. J Neurosci. 2010;30:11128–42.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  98. Newman EL, Gillet SN, Climer JR, Hasselmo ME. Cholinergic blockade reduces theta-gamma phase amplitude coupling and speed modulation of theta frequency consistent with behavioral effects on encoding. J Neurosci. 2013;33:19635–46.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  99. Pernía-Andrade AJ, Jonas P. Theta-gamma-modulated synaptic currents in hippocampal granule cells in vivo define a mechanism for network oscillations. Neuron. 2014;81:140–52.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  100. Yamamoto J, Suh J, Takeuchi D, Tonegawa S. Successful execution of working memory linked to synchronized high-frequency gamma oscillations. Cell. 2014;157:845–57.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Cunningham MO, Davies CH, Buhl EH, Kopell N, Whittington MA. Gamma oscillations induced by kainate receptor activation in the entorhinal cortex in vitro. J Neurosci. 2003;23:9761–9.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Goutagny R, Gu N, Cavanagh C, Jackson J, Chabot J-G, Quirion R, Krantic S, Williams S. Alterations in hippocampal network oscillations and theta-gamma coupling arise before A-beta overproduction in a mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. Eur J Neurosci. 2013;37:1896–902.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  103. Pastoll H, Solanka L, van Rossum MC, Nolan MF. Feedback inhibition enables theta-nested gamma oscillations and grid firing fields. Neuron. 2013;77:141–54.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  104. Lakatos P, Shah AS, Knuth KH, Ulbert I, Karmos G, Schroeder CE. An oscillatory hierarchy controlling neuronal excitability and stimulus processing in the auditory cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2005;94:1904–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  105. Voloh B, Valiante TA, Everling S, Womelsdorf T. Theta-gamma coordination between anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex indexes correct attention shifts. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2015;112:8457–62.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  106. Fell J, Klaver P, Elfadil H, Schaller C, Elger CE, Fernández G. Rhinal-hippocampal theta coherence during declarative memory formation: interaction with gamma synchronization? Eur J Neurosci. 2003;17:1082–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  107. Van der Meij R, Kahana M, Maris E. Phase-amplitude coupling in human electrocorticography is spatially distributed and phase diverse. J Neurosci. 2012;32:111–23.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  108. Maris E, van Vugt M, Kahana M. Spatially distributed patterns of oscillatory coupling between high-frequency amplitudes and low-frequency phases in human iEEG. Neuroimage. 2011;54:836–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  109. Schack B, Weiss S. Quantification of phase synchronization phenomena and their importance for verbal memory processes. Biol Cybern. 2005;92:275–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  110. Lee Y-Y, Yang C-Y. Utilizing the extent of theta-gamma synchronization to estimate visuospatial memory ability. Australas Phys Eng Sci Med. 2014;37:665–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  111. Vosskuhl J, Huster RJ, Herrmann CS. Increase in short-term memory capacity induced by down-regulating individual theta frequency via transcranial alternating current stimulation. Front Hum Neurosci. 2015;9:257.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  112. Alekseichuk I, Turi Z, de Lara GA, Antal A, Paulus W. Spatial Working Memory in Humans Depends on Theta and High Gamma Synchronization in the Prefrontal Cortex. Curr Biol. 2016;26:1513–21.

  113. Schmiedt C, Brand A, Hildebrandt H, Başar-Eroğlu C. Event-related theta oscillations during working memory tasks in patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls. Cogn Brain Res. 2005;25:936–47.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  114. Missonnier P, Herrmann FR, Zanello A, Bâ MB, Curtis L, Canovas D, Chantraine F, Richiardi J, Giannakopoulos P, Merlo MC. Event-related potentials and changes of brain rhythm oscillations during working memory activation in patients with first-episode psychosis. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2012;37:95.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  115. Griesmayr B, Berger B, Stelzig-Schoeler R, Aichhorn W, Bergmann J, Sauseng P. EEG theta phase coupling during executive control of visual working memory investigated in individuals with schizophrenia and in healthy controls. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2014;14:1340–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  116. Kissler J, Müller MM, Fehr T, Rockstroh B, Elbert T. MEG gamma band activity in schizophrenia patients and healthy subjects in a mental arithmetic task and at rest. Clin Neurophysiol. 2000;111:2079–87.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  117. Başar-Eroğlu C, Brand A, Hildebrandt H, Kedzior KK, Mathes B, Schmiedt C. Working memory related gamma oscillations in schizophrenia patients. Int J Psychophysiol. 2007;64:39–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  118. Barr M, Farzan F, Tran LC, Chen R, Fitzgerald P, Daskalakis Z. Evidence for excessive frontal evoked gamma oscillatory activity in schizophrenia during working memory. Schizophr Res. 2010;121:146–52.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  119. Chen C-MA, Stanford AD, Mao X, Abi-Dargham A, Shungu DC, Lisanby SH, Schroeder CE, Kegeles LS. GABA level, gamma oscillation, and working memory performance in schizophrenia. NeuroImage. 2014;4:531–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  120. Sternberg S. In defence of high-speed memory scanning. Q J Exp Psychol. 2016;69:2020–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are grateful for the commentary of this paper’s reviewers, whose recommendations greatly improved upon the original manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by grants to Scott Sponheim from the National Institute of Mental Health (R03MH106831) and from the Veterans Health Administration Clinical Science Research and Development Program (ICX000227A).

Availability of data and materials

Data not shared: not applicable (review of existing literature).

Authors’ contributions

PAL wrote the submitted manuscript; SRS advised writing of the manuscript and carried out editing of the submission. Both authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Competing interests

Both authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Scott R. Sponheim.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lynn, P.A., Sponheim, S.R. Disturbed theta and gamma coupling as a potential mechanism for visuospatial working memory dysfunction in people with schizophrenia. Neuropsychiatr Electrophysiol 2, 7 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40810-016-0022-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40810-016-0022-3

Keywords